Working memory in school children with dyslexia A relational analysis

Main Article Content

Catalina Quintero-López
Víctor-Daniel Gil-Vera
Laura Bolívar-Villamil
Kelin-Camila Mazo-Benítez
Mariana Serna-Jaramillo
Laura-María Ciro-Graciano
Kathelyn-Charlot Restrepo-Arias

Abstract

Working memory (WM) is an essential cognitive process for the acquisition of learning in reading, writing and arithmetic, allowing for the proper processing of environmental stimuli; people with dyslexia (DLX) have alterations in this neurocognitive process, mainly in handling verbal information.  The objective of this study is to analyse the link between MT, verbal memory, visual/verbal memory, consciousness level, mind control and semantic memory in a sample of 130 schoolchildren diagnosed with DLX. In order to identify the link between the aforementioned variables, a structural equation model (SEM) was constructed in RCran 4.0.4 software, using the results obtained in the psychometric tests applied; WISC-IV, Visual/Verbal Memory, Verbal Memory and Wechsler Memory Curve. This paper concludes that people with DLX have deficiencies in several memory domains, the neuropsychological stimulation of this process is fundamental to guarantee the scholastic progress of the population with this neurodevelopmental disorder.

Article Details

How to Cite
Quintero-López, C., Gil-Vera, V.-D. ., Bolívar-Villamil, . L., Mazo-Benítez, K.-C. ., Serna-Jaramillo, M., Ciro-Graciano, L.-M. ., & Restrepo-Arias, K.-C. . (2022). Working memory in school children with dyslexia: A relational analysis. Ocnos. Journal of reading research, 21(2). https://doi.org/10.18239/ocnos_2022.21.2.2886
Section
Artículos
Quintero-López, Gil-Vera, Bolívar-Villamil, Mazo-Benítez, Serna-Jaramillo, Ciro-Graciano, and Restrepo-Arias: Working memory in school children with dyslexia. A relational analysis

Introduction

According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [DSM-V] (2013) neurodevelopmental disorders (2013), they are early-onset clinical disturbances, which break out before school age and affect the educational, social and family sphere; they have a negative impact on academic learning, executive functions, social skills and intelligence. (; ). Specific learning disorder falls within this group of clinical entities, characterised by impairment in the acquisition of academic skills in reading, writing or arithmetic (). It has been referred to as dyscalculia (problems in learning mathematical skills), dysgraphia (impaired writing expression) and DLX (difficulties in learning to read). (; ). There is a higher prevalence of DLX in the school population, reported to be between 5% and 15% (; ). People diagnosed therewith show inaccuracy in reading words, poor lightness, poor understanding of texts, and poor reading comprehension, problems in pronunciation and visual processing (). Profiling studies indicate that DLX is associated with neurocognitive impairments. (; ; ). WM is essential to acquire reading, writing and mathematical learning (; ; ). There is low performance on neuropsychological tests assessing verbal WM in people with DLX, research findings have reported significant alterations in this neurocognitive process (; ). WM allows for the proper manipulation of the medium's information through encoding, processing and immediate recall (; ; ). Baddeley’s approach has been a significant clinical and scientific benchmark for theorising WM (central executive), made up of by the articulatory loop and the visuospatial agenda (; ). The visuospatial agenda is committed to the performance and sustainment of visual/spatial information; the articulatory loop is the verbal memory responsible for receiving information from the environment and the linguistic cognitive system, and maintaining the phonological codes for a short period of time (; ; ). Research has concluded that WM deficits in patients diagnosed with DLX are found in verbal memory, associated with problems in level of awareness, mind control, slow speed in naming verbal and visual symbols affecting the processing and storage of phonological material (; ; ). Semantic memory is involved in spontaneously acquired knowledge about the environment, it directly influences the functionality of WM, in the pedagogical context it allows the manipulation of linguistic codes for the acquisition of literacy skills. (; ).

There is a wide range of research indicating that people with DLX have impairments in memory components, mainly in WM (, ; ), but there is little scientific evidence analysing the relationship between these components; these findings motivate the following research question: What is the link between WM, verbal memory, visual/verbal memory, consciousness level, mind control and semantic memory in schoolchildren with DLX? Deficits in this neurocognitive component should be addressed clinically, fostering this population’s educational progress ().

Based on the above, the aim of this research was to analyse the link between WM, verbal memory, visual/verbal memory, consciousness level, mind control and semantic memory in schoolchildren with DLX. The following relational research hypotheses were formulated: H1. In people with DLX, the lower the WM functionality, the greater the impairment of verbal memory, H2. In people with DLX, the lower the WM functionality, the greater the deficits in visual/verbal memory, H3. The greater the alterations in consciousness level, the lower the functionality of mind control and H4. The lower mind control, the greater the deficiencies in semantic memory. It was concluded that in DLX, WM is affected and is directly related to the clinical manifestations of this neurodevelopmental disorder.

Method

Participants

From a population of 156 patients with neurodevelopmental disorders, patients (N=130) diagnosed with DLX were included in the sample. The 26 who were excluded had sensory deficits or intellectual disabilities. The sample under analyses was made up of primary/secondary school children, ranging in age from 7 to 15 years. 82 males (Mage=9.30, SDage=2.18, right-handed=75, left-handed= 7, ambidextrous= 0) and 48 females (Mage=9.27, SDage=2.23, right-handed= 42, left-handed= 5, ambidextrous= 1). The medical records of each patient were analysed to identify the presence/absence of neurological risk, family history of DLX or deficiencies in successive developmental milestones (17 patients with neurological risk and delays in successive developmental milestones, 26 patients with neurological risk and no delays in successive developmental milestones, 23 patients with neurological risk and no neurological risk, 64 patients with no neurological risk and no delays in successive developmental milestones).

Design

The study had a quantitative approach, the level was relational, non-experimental, cross-sectional design ().

Instruments

Wechsler Intelligence Scale -Wechsler- (WISC-IV) (). It makes a comprehensive assessment of cognitive/intellectual functioning and is administered among individuals aged between 6 and 16. It is made up of 10 primary tests and 5 supplementary tests. For this research, the tests measuring the WM index; letter/number succession-digit retention, which assesses recording and data retention skills, were applied. Letter/number succession, estimates storage and combination capabilities of various types of information; the test taker is read a set of letters and numbers, then asked to output the letters in the order of the alphabet and the numbers in an ascending arrangement. The maximum score of this cognitive activity is 30; it includes 10 items each made up of 3 items, and the test is terminated when the test taker has 3 scores of zero on the same item. Digit retention, examines sequential skills, organisation, alertness and cognitive flexibility of WM. It includes two activities; digits in direct arrangement (the test taker repeats the numbers in the same arrangement as presented by the tester), digits in reverse arrangement (the test taker repeats the numbers in the opposite arrangement as presented by the tester). It is made up of 8 items, each with two items; a score of zero on both items of an item is a fail. Each test’s score involves the conversion of direct scores into scalar scores. The reliability of the scale was tested through test-retest, it has content validity (; ).

Wechsler Memory Scale- (). Administered individually, this test allows for the assessment of the fundamental aspects of memory (information, orientation, mind control, logical memory and associated pairs) in different age groups. The information and orientation test assess the level of awareness through very simple questions about personal elements, basic general culture, location in space and time. Mind Control, made up of three activities, aimed at assessing automated and controlled processing (counting backwards from twenty to one, repeating the alphabet and counting from 3 to 3 starting from 4 up to 40). Logical memory, made up of two stories that are read to the person being tested for immediate recall. Associated pairs, consists on introducing the patient to a pair of words to be memorised, and then telling him to remember the second word while the first word is presented. The version validated by for Colombian children and adolescents was applied to the sample. The test has appropriate construct and content validity.

Verbal Memory Curve (). The test taker is given 10 words sequentially and must recall the words he remembers immediately after each presentation. Up to 10 verbalisations and evocations of the words are allowed, aiming a certain maximum volume. The score obtained from the initial volume indicates the number of items that the person tested remembers after the first exposure. The maximum volume refers to the number of items that can be evoked after up to 10 training sessions. The test has adequate validity and reliability.

The Visual/Verbal Memory Scale (). A template with 10 drawings is presented for the person tested to say the name of each drawing, then the visual template is removed so that they can immediately recall the ones they remember, the test allows for up to 10 trials. Initial volume, assesses the number of words evoked after the first trial; peak volume, assesses the maximum number of words evoked after up to 10 trials. The test has adequate validity and reliability.

Procedure

This study was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Universidad Católica Luis Amigó under file number 62542. At the Neuropser Neuropsychology Specialised Care Centre (Medellín-Colombia), the sample of primary and secondary school children between the ages of 7 and 15 years with a diagnosis of DLX was assessed. The supervisors of the population assessed agreed to sign the appropriate informed consent form for the minors to participate in the research project. The application, scoring and interpretation of the test protocol provided was carried out by a team of professionals specialised in neuropsychology; the assessment of each participant was carried out individually, with an average of two hours. This research was financed by the Universidad Católica Luis Amigó under code 0502029969 of the Cost Centre.

Data analysis

The sample descriptives and SEM were performed within the RCran 4.0.4 environment for statistical computing. Covariance was analysed between the latent variables; WM verbal memory, visual/verbal memory, consciousness level, mind control and semantic memory of the tests provided. SEMs allow the validation of hypothesised constructs between latent variables from observable variables, they are a class of multivariate structures (). These models determine the dependence or independence relationship by integrating linear equations, combining factor analysis with linear regression to calculate the data fit. Goodness-of-fit tests were performed; Chi-Square, the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker Lewis Index and the p-value of indicators.

Results

Table 1 presents the summary of descriptive statistics for the characterisation of the patients under analysis. It shows that there are more men than women (82>48), age was similar for both sexes. Participants in the sample were aged between 7 and 15 years. Most patients were right-handed, the number of left-handed patients was the same in both sexes (7 patients), only one female patient was ambidextrous. The patients’ level of education ranged from first grade of primary school to ninth grade of high school. 32% of patients were at neurological risk and 68% were not. 30% of the patients under analysis lagged behind in the successive evolution of the developmental areas. Neurological risk represents adverse factors in the pregnancy/delivery process and family history of DLX; deficiencies in developmental areas present divergences between chronological age and expected developmental milestones (; ; ).

Gender No. Age RN RAD Laterality
D Z AD
x ¯ σ 2
Man 82 9.30 2.18 Yes= 27 No=55 Yes= 20 No=62 75 7 0
Woman 48 9.27 2.23 Yes=15 No=33 Yes=19 No=29 42 5 1
Total 130 Yes=42 No=88 Yes=39 No= 91 117 12 1
Table 1 Descriptive statistics  

The LAVAAN library was used to build the SEM, the model converged after 95 iterations. The number of degrees of freedom of the SEM was 30, the Chi-square statistic was greater than 503, defining an adequate model. The Comparative Fit Index CFI=0.856 > 0.5, as well as the Tucker Lewis Index TLI=0.737 > 0.5, indicate the relevance of the SEM. The p-value of the indicators suggests significance, the model developed is valid. All the values of the covariances analysed were positive, accepting the hypotheses put forward. Table 2 presents the results of the SEM.

Assumption Covariance Criterion P(>|z|)
H1. In people with DLX, the lower the WM functionality,the greater the impairment of verbal memory 0.72 Accepts 0,000
H2. In people with DLX, the lower the WM functionality,the greater the deficits in visual/verbal memory 0.51 Accepts 0,000
H3. The greater the alterations in consciousness level,the lower the functionality of mind control. 0.55 Accepts 0,000
H4. The lower mind control, the greater thedeficiencies in semantic memory 0.55 Accepts 0,000
Table 2 Relational hypotheses  

Figure 1 presents the diagram of the developed SEM. The numerical value above the two-way arrows indicates the value of the covariance. Variables between rectangles indicate observed variables, variables between ovals indicate latent variables.

2886_gf2.png
Figure 1 Structural Equation Model   Note. Verbal Memory (Mmr Vr), Visual and Verbal Memory (Mmr Vs), Consciousness Level (N) and Mind Control (C_M), Semantic Memory (M_S)

Discussion

According to epidemiological data, DLX is more prevalent in men (). The sample analysed in this research was mainly made up of men. Some predisposing factors for DLX have been identified that are not determinant; neurological risk, hereditary background and delay in developmental areas (). In the sample analysed, 62% had predisposing factors, which shows that not all people diagnosed with DLX have neurological risk or deficiencies in developmental areas.

From the results of the SEM in the DLX there is a deficit in WM, which directly affects verbal memory. This finding is consistent with the results of some research that suggests that DLX generates abnormalities in verbal memory, affecting the functioning of the phonological component of WM, manifesting in perceptual and linguistic difficulties (; ; ). It was evidenced that people with DLX present alterations in the processing of visual/verbal information, implying impairments in visual memory, a slowing down in the processing of data captured from the environment; attention operates in an articulated manner with WM for the automated processing of stimuli. From the scientific background review, little research validates this claim, and it was found that DLX generates deficits in visual processing, negatively impacting the performance of tasks that assess WM (; ; ).

Schoolchildren with DLX show failures in tasks that measure mind control, they have a lower ability to focus attention, they do not have adequate competences to regulate the information they receive from the environment due to inhibitory control failures (). This research finding is consistent with hypothesis H3 stated in this research. Attention and WM are fundamental for mind control, cognitive processes that facilitate the learning of pedagogical competences. (; ). The impairment in mental control linked to DLX has an impact on semantic memory functionality (). argue that deficiencies in WM and its link with verbal and visual memory and mental control lead to a decrease in semantic storage, with reduced orthographic processing, especially in the early stages of school, adversely impacting academic learning. Studies show that DLX is due to an impaired pattern of recall (episodic/semantic), has been attributed mainly to impaired phonological processing, suggests a conceptual link between reading difficulties and phonological and orthographic integration deficits. Early intervention for language deficits reduces the risk of WM limitations, a key component of good reading and writing performance. (; ; ).

It is concluded that WM is impaired in patients diagnosed with DLX mainly at the level of verbal memory (articulatory loop) and mental control. In this clinical condition there are deficits in explicit memory affecting semantic memory and visual and verbal memory. The SEM developed allowed us to identify that in people with DLX the deficiencies in WM are related to verbal, visual and controlled processing alterations, generating and maintaining some of the symptoms associated with this clinical condition (slow learning, difficulties in morpheme formation, problems in the storage and evocation of phonemes). Studies assessing visual and verbal memory in people with DLX are suggested, the scientific evidence is not conclusive. The results of this research are limited to Spanish-speaking people with DLX. It is important that psychology and neuropsychology continue to explore the link between WM and DLX in order to foster specialised intervention processes for this population.

References

1 

Acha, J. (2016). Hacia un modelo multidimensional del trastorno específico del lenguaje y la dislexia: Déficits compartidos y específicos. Revista de Investigación en Logopedia, 6(2), 107-141. https://doi.org/10.5209/rlog.58545

2 

Arnett, A. B., Pennington, B. F., Peterson, R. L., Willcutt, E. G., DeFries, J. C., & Olson, R. K. (2017). Explaining the sex difference in dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(6), 719-727. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12691

3 

American Psychiatric Association - APA. (2013). Manual Diagnóstico y Estadístico de los Trastornos Mentales DSM-5 (5th. Ed.). Editorial Médica Panamericana.

4 

Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., Guirado-Moreno, J. L., Sánchez-Azanza, V., Adrover-Roig, D., & Valera-Pozo, M. (2021). Intervención en el control inhibitorio en niños con y sin trastorno de lenguaje dentro del aula. Revista de Investigación en Logopedia, 11, 115-128. https://doi.org/10.5209/rlog.69256

5 

Albano, D., García, R. B., & Cornoldi, C. (2016). Deficits in working memory visual-phonological binding in children with dyslexia. Psychology & Neuroscience, 9(4), 411-419. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000066

6 

Ardila, A., Rosselli, M., & Puente, A. (1994). Neuropsychological evaluation of the Spanish Speaker. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1453-8

7 

Ardila, A., & Rosselli, M. (1994). Development of language, memory, and visuospatial abilities in 5- to 12-year-old children using a neuropsychological battery. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10(2), 97-120. https://doi.org/10.1080/87565649409540571

8 

Ardila, F., & Rosselli, M. (2007). Neuropsicología clínica. Manual Moderno

9 

Artuso, C., Bellelli, F., & Belacchi, C. (2020). Developmental dyslexia: How taxonomic and thematic organization affect working memory recall. Child Neuropsychology, 26(2), 242-256. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2019.1640869

10 

Bausela-Herreras, E., Tirapu-Ustárroz, J., Cordero-Andrés, P. (2019). Déficits ejecutivos y trastornos del neurodesarrollo en la infancia y en la adolescencia. Revista Neurología, 69(11), 461-469. https://doi.org/10.33588/rn.6911.2019133

11 

Batista, M., & Pestun, M. S. V. (2019). Modelo RTI como estratégia de prevenção aos transtornos de aprendizagem. Psicología Escolar y Educacional, 23, 1-8. https://doi.org/10.1590/2175-35392019015929

12 

Bajre, P., & Khan, A. (2019). Developmental dyslexia in Hindi readers: Is consistent sound‐symbol mapping an asset in reading? Evidence from phonological and visuospatial working memory. Dyslexia, 25(4), 390-410. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1632

13 

Brooks, A. D., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2011). Letter naming and letter writing reversals in children with dyslexia: Momentary inefficiency in the phonological and orthographic loops of working memory. Developmental neuropsychology, 36(7), 847-868. https://doi.org//10.1080/87565641.2011.606401

14 

Carballal-Mariño, M., Gago-Ageitos, A., Ares-Alvarez, J., Del Rio-Garma, M., García-Cendón, C., Goicoechea-Castaño, A., Pena-Nieto, J. (2018). Prevalence of neurodevelopmental, behavioural and learning disorders in Pediatric Primary Care. Anales de Pediatría, 89(3), 153-161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anpedi.2017.10.007

15 

Campos-Millán, L. (2019). Conceptos ad hoc, arquitectura cognitiva y localismo léxico. Tópicos, Revista De Filosofía, (57), 125-148. https://doi.org//10.21555/top.v0i57.1007

16 

Canet-Juric, L., Stelzer, F., Andrés, M. L., Vernucci, S., Introzzi, I., & Burin, D. (2018). Evidencias de validez de una tarea computarizada de memoria de trabajo verbal y viso-espacial para niños. Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 52(1), 112-128. https://doi.org/10.30849/rip/ijp.v52i1.356

17 

Cruz-Rodrigues, C., Barbosa, T., Toledo-Piza, C. M. J., Miranda, M. C., & Amodeo Bueno., O. F. (2014). Neuropsychological characteristics of dyslexic children. Psicología: Reflexão e Crítica, 27(3), 539-546. https://doi.org/10.1590/1678-7153.201427315

18 

Dawes, E., Leitão, S., Claessen, M., & Nayton, M. (2015). A Profile of Working Memory Ability in Poor Readers. Australian Psychologist, 50(5), 362-371. https://doi.org/10.1111/ap.12120

19 

Delage, H., & Durrleman, S. (2018). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: distinct syntactic profiles? Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 32(8), 758-785. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2018.1437222

20 

De-La-Peña, C., & Bernabéu, E. (2018). Dislexia y discalculia: una revisión sistemática actual desde la neurogenética. Universitas Psychologica, 17(3), 1-11. https://doi.org/10.11144/Javeriana.upsy17-3.ddrs

21 

De la Peña-Álvarez, C. (2014). Inteligencia verbal y memoria verbal en escolares disléxicos de primaria. REOP, Revista Española de Orientación y Psicopedagogía, 23(3), 81-95. https://doi.org/10.5944/reop.vol.23.num.3.2012.11463

22 

Fogel, Y., Josman, N., & Rosenblum, S. (2021). Exploring the Impacts of Environmental Factors on Adolescents’ Daily Participation: A Structural Equation Modelling Approach. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(1), 142. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18010142

23 

González-Valenzuela, M.-J., & Martin-Ruiz, I. (2016). Intervención temprana del desarrollo del lenguaje oral en niños en riesgo de dificultades de aprendizaje: Un estudio longitudinal. Revista Mexicana de Psicología, 33(1), 50-60. https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=243056043006

24 

Gráf, R., Kalmár, M., Harnos, A., Boross, G., & Nagy, A. (2020). Reading and spelling skills of prematurely born children in light of the underlying cognitive factors. Cognitive Processing, 22, 311-319. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-020-01001-6

25 

Gray, S., Fox, A. B., Green, S., Alt, M., Hogan, T. P., Petscher, Y., & Cowan, N. (2019). Working Memory Profiles of Children with Dyslexia, Developmental Language Disorder, or Both. Journal of speech, language, and hearing research: JSLHR, 62(6), 1839-1858. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-18-0148

26 

Giorgetti, M., & Lorusso, M. (2018). Specific conditions for a selective deficit in memory for order in children with dyslexia. Child Neuropsychology, 25, 1-30. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2018.1530746

27 

Gutiérrez-Sánchez, M., & Vidal-Valenzuela, S. (2019). La Escala Observacional de Memoria Operativa (EOMO) como instrumento eficaz en la prevención y detección de dificultades de aprendizaje. Revista de Investigación Educativa, 38(1), 53-69. http://doi.org/10.6018/rie.313271

28 

Irak, M., Turan, G., Güler, B., & Orgun, Z. (2019). Investigating memory functions in dyslexia and other specific learning disorders. Life Span and Disability, 22(2), 223-253.

29 

Jiménez, J. E., Morales, C., & Rodríguez, C. (2014). Subtipos disléxicos y procesos fonológicos y ortográficos en la escritura de palabras. European Journal of Education and Psychology, 7(1), 5-16. https://doi.org/10.30552/ejep.v7i1.101

30 

Kapa, L. L., & Plante, E. (2015). Executive function in sli: recent advances and future directions. Current developmental disorders reports, 2(3), 245-252. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-015-0050-x

31 

Kibby, M. Y., Lee, S. E., & Dyer, S. M. (2014). Reading performance is predicted by more than phonological processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(960), 1-7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00960

32 

Lah, S., & Smith, M. L. (2014). Semantic and episodic memory in children with temporal lobe epilepsy: Do they relate to literacy skills? Neuropsychology, 28(1), 113-122. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000029

33 

Lawton, T. (2016). Improving Dorsal Stream Function in Dyslexics by Training Figure/Ground Motion Discrimination Improves Attention, Reading Fluency, and Working Memory. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 1-16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00397

34 

Lazzaro, G., Varuzza, C., Costanzo, F., Fucà, E., Di Vara, S., De Matteis, M. E., Vicari, S., & Menghini, D. (2021). Memory Deficits in Children with Developmental Dyslexia: A Reading-Level and Chronological-Age Matched Design. Brain Sciences, 11(1), 40. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11010040

35 

Maehler, C., Joerns, C., & Schuchardt, K. (2019). Training Working Memory of Children with and without Dyslexia. Children, 6(3), 47. https://doi.org/10.3390/children6030047

36 

Männel, C., Meyer, L., Wilcke, A., Boltze, J., Kirsten, H., & Friederici, A. D. (2015). Working-memory endophenotype and dyslexia-associated genetic variant predict dyslexia phenotype. Cortex, 71, 291-305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.029

37 

Martins, R. A., Ribeiro, M. G., Pastura, G. M. C., & Monteiro, M. C. (2020). Remediação fonológica em escolares com TDAH e dislexia. Remediação fonológica em escolares com TDAH e dislexia. CoDAS, 32(5). https://doi.org/10.1590/2317-1782/20192019086

38 

Masoura, E., Gogou, A., & Gathercole, S. E. (2020). Working memory profiles of children with reading difficulties who are learning to read in Greek. Dyslexia, 27(3), 312-324. https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1671

39 

Medina-Alva, M.P., Caro-Kahn, I., Muñoz-Huerta, P., Leyva-Sánchez, J., Moreno-Calixto, J., & Vega-Sánchez, S.M. (2015). Neurodesarrollo infantil: características normales y signos de alarma en el niño menor de cinco años. Revista Peruana de Medicina Experimental y Salud Pública, 32(3), 565-573. https://doi.org/10.17843/rpmesp.2015.323.1693

40 

Medina, G. B. K., & Guimarães, S. R. K. (2021). Reading in developmental dyslexia: the role of phonemic awareness and executive functions. Estudos de Psicologia (Campinas), 38, e180178. https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-0275202138e180178

41 

Jalali-Moghadam, N., & Kormi-Nouri, R. (2017). Bilingualism and reading difficulties: an exploration in episodic and semantic memory. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 29(5), 570-582. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2017.1293673

42 

Ortiz, R., Estévez, A., & Muñetón, M. (2014). El procesamiento temporal en la percepción del habla de los disléxicos. Annals of Psychology, 30(2), 716-724. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.30.2.151261

43 

Pham, A. V., & Riviere, A. (2015). Specific Learning Disorders and ADHD: Current Issues in Diagnosis Across Clinical and Educational Settings. Current psychiatry reports, 17(6), 38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-015-0584-y

44 

Rodrigues, J. C., da Fontoura, D. R., & de Salles, J. F. (2014). Acquired dysgraphia in adults following right or left-hemisphere stroke. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 8(3), 236-242. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1980-57642014DN83000007

45 

Romero-González, M., Lavigne-Cerván, R., Sánchez-Muñoz de León, M., Gamboa-Ternero, S., Juárez-Ruiz de Mier, R., & Romero-Pérez, J. F. (2021). Effects of a Home Literacy Environment Program on Psychlinguistic Variables in Children from 6 to 8 Years of Age. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 18(6), 3085. http://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18063085

46 

Rousselle, M., & Abadie, M. (2021). La mémoire de travail dans la dyslexie: Dysfonctionnements et pistes de remédiation. ANAE Approche neuropsychologique des apprentissages chez l’enfant, 171, 154-161.

47 

Sánchez-Gómez, M. C., Rodrigues, A. I. & Costa, A. P. (2018). Desde los métodos cualitativos hacia los modelos mixtos: tendencia actual de investigación en ciencias sociales. RISTI - Revista Ibérica de Sistemas e Tecnologías de Informação, 28, 9-13. https://doi.org/10.17013/risti.28.0

48 

Soares, C. S., Guerra, A., Roy, A., Hazin, I., & Azoni, C. S. (2020). Developmental dyslexia and executive functions: evidence on main evaluation methods. Estudos de Psicologia (Natal), 25(1), 1-9. https://doi.org/10.17013/risti.28.0

49 

Stošljević, M., Odović, G. & Adamović, M. (2012). Integral treatment of children with dyslexia: 40 years’ experience. Srpski arhiv za celokupno lekarstvo, 140(9), 625-629. https://doi.org/10.2298/SARH1210625S

50 

Tamayo-Lorenzo, S. (2017). La dislexia y las dificultades en la adquisición de la lectoescritura. Revista de Currículum y Formación de Profesorado, 21(1), 423-432. https://www.redalyc.org/pdf/567/56750681021.pdf

51 

Toffalini, E., Tomasi, E., Albano, D., & Cornoldi, C. (2018). The effects of the constancy of location and order in working memory visual-phonological binding of children with dyslexia. Child neuropsychology: A journal on normal and abnormal development in childhood and adolescence, 24(5), 671-685. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2017.1329411

52 

Yang, J., Peng, J., Zhang, D., Zheng, L., & Mo, L. (2017). Specific effects of working memory training on the reading skills of Chinese children with developmental dyslexia. PLOSONE, 12(11), e0186114. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0186114

53 

Varvara, P., Varuzza, C., Sorrentino, A., Vicari, S., & Menghini, D. (2014). Executive functions in developmental dyslexia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(120), 1-8. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffnhum.2014.00120

54 

Vericat, A., & Orden, A.B. (2017). Riesgo neurológico en el niño de mediano riesgo neonatal. Acta Pediátrica de México, 38(4), 255-266. https://doi.org/10.18233/APM38No4pp255-2661434

55 

Velasteguí-Egüez, J. E., Hernández-Navarro, M. I., Real-Cotto, J. J., Roby Arias, A. J., Alvarado-Franco, H.J., & Haro-Velastegui, A.J. (2018). Complicaciones perinatales asociadas al embarazo en adolescentes de Atacames. Revista Cubana de Medicina General Integral, 34(1), 37-44. http://scielo.sld.cu/pdf/mgi/v34n1/mgi05118.pdf

56 

Wang, H. C, Nickels, L., & Castles, A. (2015). Orthographic learning in developmental surface and phonological dyslexia. Cognitive neuropsychology, 32(2), 58-79. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643294.2014.1003536

57 

Wechsler, D. (1945). A Standardized Memory Scale for Clinical Use. The Journal of Psychology, 19(1), 87–95. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1945.9917223

58 

Wechsler, D. (2005). WISC-IV: Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler para Niños-IV. TEA

59 

Werth, R. (2019). What causes dyslexia? Identifying the causes and effective compensatory therapy. Restorative neurology and neuroscience, 37(6), 591-608. https://doi.org/10.3233/rnn-190939

60 

Wiseheart, R., & Altmann, L. J. P. (2017). Spoken sentence production in college students with dyslexia: working memory and vocabulary effects. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 53(2), 355-369. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12353

Notes

[i] The results of the SEM adjustment are available online at the following URL: https://github.com/victorgil777/MEE/blob/35eb9934ff4e2d2956aa2c3ca9bef4f06e5637f0/MT