This article presents the results of a research developed about the narrations in digital platforms of the picture book The bear under the stairs, by Helen Cooper (1993). In this picture book we find a word-image interaction which sometimes causes a counterpoint relationship. On the other hand, it starts and ends the narration within the margins of the text, within the spaces reserved for the peritext. This research studies how mediators from different countries, mother tongues, and training narrate this book in particular in digital platforms. Of a sample of 53 videos in Spanish, English, and Catalan, we analyzed the strategies used by these digital mediators to reveal or conceal the cues that enable the reader to interpret the work, as well as the interpretations derived from these strategies and the amplifications made, which propose either enriching or reductive readings.
Article Details
How to Cite
Quesada-Padrón, C., Díaz-Armas, J., Perdomo-López, C. A., & Mauclair, P. (2025). Enriched and reductive interpretations in the digital narration of picture books. Ocnos. Journal of reading research, 24(1). https://doi.org/10.18239/ocnos_2025.24.1.477
Quesada-Padrón, Díaz-Armas, Perdomo-López, and Mauclair: Enriched and reductive interpretations in the digital narration of picture books
Introduction
Although Gerard Genette, in his study of 1982 regarding transtextual relations, included
illustrations among the possible paratexts within a work, in picture books, the illustration
cannot be considered to be an accessory, but part of one whole multimodal text (; ; ). If picture books are to be considered as a whole in which word and image are not
the only pieces that form them (), in this work we will consider the relationships between word and image according
to different explicative models (Mourão, ; ; ; ), from interrelation (for which some terms such as collaboration or interdependency,
among others, are proposed), to the substitution of the literary text by images in
silent books, and the relations of contradiction or counterpoint (; ; ).
This last type of relationship obliges the reader to rebuild the direction of the
text when perceiving the existence of two different narrators that disagree with each
other and transmit different versions of the same event: the literary narrator that
“pronounces” the words and the visual narrator that shows what is happening (; ; ; ).
Genette indicated in 1982 that the elements located on the threshold of the text are
responsible for the reading contract as bearers, at least, of architextual information
(). Throughout that passage of Palimpsestes he is referring to the peritext, although in that moment he is only using the term
paratext, which would some years later be divided in epitext and peritext ().
As picture books are, to begin with, a book whose recipient is a child reader, their
peritext present singularities such as the frequent absence of elements that can be
found in other books (index, introductions, prologues, epilogues, names of chapters,
footnotes or endnotes, pagination), as well as the importance acquired by images and
texts included in the spaces that form the peritext: cover and back cover, endpapers,
title page or copyright page.
The spaces of the peritext in picture books have received meticulous attention (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ; ), and there are studies that elaborate on the typology and the possibilities offered
by covers and back covers (Sipe & Brightman, 2005; ), dust jackets and belly bands (; ), endpapers (; ; Sipe & McGuire, ; ), title pages (), barcodes (), the picture book design (Ramos, ; ) or the typography (; ; ).
Some studies have highlighted a functionality in the elements of the peritext that
goes beyond the anticipation, the activation of the reading process, or the formation
of the reading contract. A characteristic that is singular regarding picture books
-present also in other artistic texts characterised by narrativity, as in film, where
it is increasingly more popular to find post-credit scenes- is the use of spaces of
the peritext to initiate or continue the narration or conclude it. Regarding this
characteristic, that is a growing tendency in picture books () and that can also be found in the illustrated book, where it can be used for adding
a framing narrative (). In Spanish, there have been several terms proposed by different academics: “desbordamiento
narrativo” (Díaz-Armas, ; ) −which is the one that we will use in this article− “textualización de los paratextos”
() or “hermeneutic function” (in its inclusive variant) of peritexts, added by to the function typology proposed by to analyse the peritexts in the nonfiction book (; ).
This characteristic can lead to the narration starting before the first sentence and
continuing after the last one, adding a preface or an epilogue (; Díaz-Armas, ; ); it can introduce the space and time in which the story is set in (); or incorporate “un juego o una información independiente de la historia” (), whereas the back endpapers can get to be a narrative zone where the story is concluded
or, at times, where the reader is guided back to the beginning of it (; ), or introducing a new open ending, contradicting the previous narrative (; ).
The bear under the stairs
In The bear under the stairs, by ), it has been observed () the existence of narrative information placed in areas of the book that less attentive
readers might not pay attention to (the peritext, before the first sentence and after
the last one, or some details that are less visible in the illustrations) and that
are important to perceive the existence of a relation of contradiction. The inattention
of these elements would affect, without a doubt, the reader’s construction of the
meaning of the text.
Helen Cooper’s picture book has a literary text led by an extradiegetic narrator that
suggests a conflict (William is scared of a bear that he thinks is hiding in his house),
that is resolved in the end with the overcoming of that unjustified fear, but the
illustrations show images that contradict this version:
- In the initial peritext, three illustrations narrate the moment before the appearance
of the narrative voice supported by the literary text: in the illustrations we see
how a bear disembarks a boat, waits for a bus, and comes out of it to enter a house.
- At the climactic moment of the story, when William and his mum enter the room where
the bear is hiding, the literary text affirms that in the room there was nothing but
some objects that seemed to shape the figure of a bear. In the image, the composition
is distributed in three zones: in the center we see the two humans, that direct their
gaze towards the reader’s left side, where there are several objects and utensils
piled, which seem to shape the figure of a bear: however, on the right side, hiding
in the shadow, behind the door, we can see the bear, holding some suitcases and an
umbrella, as the ones he was carrying in the initial illustrations.
- On the next page, the gaze is again decisive. William and his mum, on their knees,
clean the room under the stairs and she contemplates with a look of confusion an empty
jar, whereas on their backs, we can see how the bear is walking away from the place.
The mother’s gesture contemplating the jar is evidence for the reader to notice something
(that is the jar that, some pages before, was full of honey, the only food of all
the ones William threw that turned to be appealing for the bear).
- In the last pages, the literary text -where it affirms that William never was scared
of bears- is counterpointed by some illustrations that show the bear leaving the house
and taking a plane.
- A final illustration, without a text accompanying it, presents the moment in which
the bear descends in a parachute over a house in the suburbs.
The attention paid to these details, that are in the threshold of the text and that
are not noticed by some readers (initial and final pages, areas that are far from
the center in the composition of the illustrations), is fundamental for the construction
of meaning. A reader could notice those signs, even if those would only be useful
for reaching the conclusion that the bear is in William’s imagination (although for
that, they would have to offer an explanation that would give sense to the aspects
previously named.
Mediation and digital narration
Among the numerous studies on children readers’ response toward picture books (; ; Mourão, ; ; Pantaleo, ; ; ; Sipe & Brightman, 2005), there are many observations on how the initial peritext
helps the reader to sign a reading contract (; ).
Studies on typology and teaching practices insist on the importance of the performance
of the teacher regarding the construction of meaning by children readers (; ; ; ; ; ; ; ). To do so, it would be necessary to have an open attitude towards the students’
possible interpretations of the text (), a process that requires reexamining the elements in the peritext after reading
(; ), as in many cases, the narration starts before the first sentence appears -and it
is important that the reader notices that ()- or continues after the last one.
The narrator who reads a picture book in front of an audience has the possibility
of using their voice to clarify aspects during the reading, amplifying part of the
verbal text, or commenting on it, which would include, among other possibilities,
describing what happens in the illustrations (; ). Picture books provide resources for the oral narration, as the possibility of waiting
for the suitable moment to turn the page; of referring to parts of the books, including
the elements that are part of the peritext; or of commenting -or at least, showing-
the illustrations that add information that is not provided by the text, because “A
mere reading out aloud of the verbal text would deny the children the opportunity
to fully experience the read-aloud ‘event’ and to connect with the whole picturebook”
().
While these teaching strategies are commonly developed in educational contexts (or
libraries or other places and events related to reading mediation), they are also
frequently broadcast on radio or TV programmes. With the arrival and rise of internet,
they have persisted, transformed into digital literary texts characterized by their
multimodal communication -if the picture book is already multimodal in itself, the
web has multiplied its expressive possibilities () – asynchronous, transferred (and not autonomous), and, depending on their relationship
with the original text, either reproduced or adapted ().
This type of digital narration consists of the reproduction, shared through a platform
or social network, of a digital file in which the source literary text is preserved.
It is accompanied by a performance that is more or less faithful to the original text,
which may or may not show the printed text -in this case of a book- to the camera
and reproduces it either in a total or fragmentary way, all for a restricted or general
audience.
The reproduction of picture books through the web poses interesting challenges for
mediators that use this form of reproduction, because, in the same way we have described
an insufficient mediation in the synchronous oral narration -with the presence of
speaker and receiver in the same place- when the mediator does not allow the audience
to contemplate the illustrations or the free interpretation of a text (), these circumstances could also happen in the digital asynchronous reproduction
of a picture book.
Although there are studies on animated digital narrations (; ; ; ), the non-animated digital narrations of picture books do not seem to have received
the same attention. An exception is publication, which examines whether mediators respect or explain the silences in
picturebooks when narrating them in videos published on YouTube.
Method
Then, it seems relevant to analyse the reproductions of picture books that are present
on the web, and analyse the strategies used by mediators to show (or not) the illustrations,
especially when a) these are in the peritext, a zone that is not frequently recognized
by its readers as a carrier of narrative information, b)when the illustrations question
the reading if only the literary narration was taken into consideration, or c) when
there are details in the illustrations that are necessary for the construction of
meaning. The omissions of these aspects would cancel the ironic meaning of the story
or its ambiguity and polysemy.
To do so, we decided to study the strategies used in digital narrations of one picture
book that would gather all these characteristics. Then, we needed the concurrence
of two factors: a picture book that would gather all these aspects and that said picture
book would have enough reproductions to make it possible to draw significative conclusions.
Both requirements are found in The bear under the stairs, by .
We searched the web (on the platforms YouTube, Facebook, and Vimeo) in July 2021 (the
dates coincide with the COVID-19 pandemic, a reason why maybe the sample became more
abundant), through the words that identify the title of Helen Cooper’s picture book
in the languages known by the members in the research group, writing the title as
it was translated into said languages.
The search generated a total of 53 digital narrations in English, Spanish, and Catalan,
which, for space and legibility reasons, we separate in the document that can be consulted
in the Zenodo archive (), where we provide the fundamental data for its localization and description: the
code assigned to each video, the title, channel, platform -YouTube, Vimeo or Facebook-
duration and link.
As we intended to identify if the mediators permitted or not their audience to access
the whole polysemic richness of the work, showing or hiding what the illustration
contains, and considering that that could be done with words pronounced but also in
the pages reproduced or omitted, we studied both the visual and the verbal narration
that are shown in the video, which we call, respectively, resulting verbal text and
resulting visual text. We do so to distinguish them from the original literary and
visual text, which are part of this multimodal text that we find in Cooper’s work,
but that might have been played fragmentarily or entirely in the digital narration,
or even by amplifying (adding linguistic material in the resulting verbal text) or
by highlighting (through some way to catch the audience’s attention through the resulting
visual text, on some details that are found in the illustration).
To do so, we have collected data in a table that made it possible for us to observe
the oral text, but also the position of the camera: if the camera showed the book
or the narrator or both things; if it showed all the pages or only some of them; if
it showed the whole surface of the page, without ignoring any details or if, on the
contrary, this was made in a fragmentary way and if, during the digital narration,
the mediator interacted or not with the audience of the video.
In the resulting visual text, it was important to verify if the camera showed or hid:
- Every page of the picture book: if it included the initial and final peritext or
if, on the contrary, went from the cover to the page with the first sentence.
- Every page: if it included or excluded the pages that had only illustrations and
no words.
- All the spaces included in the page or double page: if they showed or forgot showing
details present in the threshold of the page that could contradict the literary narrator
and especially, the presence of the bear hiding in the shadow, behind the folding
screen and the door.
In the resulting verbal text, it was important to verify:
- If they made or not amplifications, adding some observation related to the actions
that were only shown in the illustrations, both in the peritext and the pages that
had illustrations and no words.
- If said amplifications, when aiming to make the reader notice the narrative overflow
and the details hidden in the page that contradict the literary narrator, added an
interpretation that was attentive to the signs that were present in the illustration,
regardless of what was the final interpretation.
We predicted that there would be, at least, two ways of not ignoring that narrative
information: the first one, by adding words to describe what is shown in the illustrations
(or referring to them with deictics: “But here…” and/or rhetoric questions: “¿Who
is this? ¿What is happening here?”); the second, making the audience notice the existence
of that information in the illustrations, though other paralinguistic signs (the voice
tone) or non-verbal: pointing them with the finger or through some other gesture.
We also predicted that the paratexts generated in these digital documents would generate
other significative data (Tabernero-Sala, 2016), so we analysed the introductions,
the credit titles, and even the comments when these were activated, to collect the
opinion of receivers and speakers, with their answers to these, in these asynchronous
narrations.
We took into consideration parameters and characteristics that can be observed in
the non-digital narrations pointed out in the previously commented works of ; , or ). However, since this was not the objective of our research, we categorized the performance
as dramatization, narration or reading, but without exploring deeply the use of gesture,
voice tones, etcetera.
The group of parameters created for analysing the videos were the following:
General information
- Platform
- Language of publication
- Place of publication (United Kingdom, USA, Spain, Latin America)
- Channel type
- Credit titles
- Number of views
- Number of “likes”
- Number of suscribers/followers
- Analysis of the comments
Communicative context
- Type of narration: addressed to the audience behind the screen or to an audience
that is located in the same scene as the narrator
- Format used for showing the pages/the book
- Production and quality of the image
- Type of production
- Quality of the image
- Background music
Strategies used in the narration
- Physical position
- Position of the book
- Simultaneity (which allows contemplating the book and the narrator at the same time)
- Narration, reading or dramatization during the reading
- Introduction and presentation before the reading
Analysis of the presence of the original linguistic text (entire or fragmentary)
- Of the words in the cover
- Of the whole literary text, from the first sentence until the last one.
Analysis of the presence of original illustrations (complete or fragmentary)
- Technique used for showing or displaying the illustrations.
○ Complete or fragmentary display
∎ Of all the pages
∎ Of the peritext: the cover
∎ Of the pages with illustrations and without text inside the book
∎ Of the threshold of the illustration (the outline and shadowed areas)
Analyisis of the resulting verbal text: substitution, reduction or amplification of:
- The peritext
○ Cover
○ Reading of the title
○ Reading of the name of the author
- Initial peritext with narrative overflow
- Final peritext with narrative overflow
- Interior pages without text
- Hidden details in the margins of some illustrations
After collecting the data using these parameters, we categorized it, and this categorization
allowed us to obtain the results that we will describe in the following section, following
the same order as the parameters mentioned above.
Results
17 out of the 53 videos were found on Facebook (32.7%), 34 on YouTube (64.15%), and
2 on Vimeo (3.7%). Nearly half were published in the United States (45%), followed
by the United Kingdom (32%), Latin America (17%), and Spain (6%). Most of the videos
(40 out of 53) were in English, 12 in Spanish, and one in Catalan. Most of these (48
out of 53) belonged to users with professional accounts managed by educational institutions,
libraries, associations, or professionals in those fields, such as teachers or librarians.
A total of 44 videos were published in 2020, and at least four featured teachers narrating
the storybook so that their students could enjoy it during the COVID-19 lockdown.
These teachers wore face masks, used encouraging phrases, recommended staying home,
and, in two cases, the videos were recorded as Zoom video calls. More than half of
the videos (28), whether from personal or professional accounts, were published by
users with channels almost exclusively dedicated to storytelling.
In July 2021, 40 of these videos had reached between 1 and 800 views, and only 8 of
them had between 1.000 and 9.000 views at that time. The number of “likes” also varied
considerably: 35 videos had fewer than 100 positive reactions, while others reached
up to 1.000.
There wasn’t much information to gather from the comments section since most of the
videos, hosted on YouTube, were marked as children’s content and were therefore protected
by blocking comments. In six of the videos, viewers mentioned that their children
enjoyed the story. Only once (in video 49) did someone disagree with the narrator’s
interpretation of the text, directly engaging with aspects related to the objectives
of our study. Specifically, in a comment on a video in Spanish, a viewer expressed
disagreement with the mediator’s interpretation of the story (claiming that the bear
was a figment of William’s imagination), and, later on, the narrator also responded
to that comment.
In more than half of the videos (57%), both the book and the narrator were shown at
the same time. Some videos (17%) used a top-down shot showing only the book’s pages
and the narrator’s hands turning them, accompanied by voice-over narration. Others
opted for slideshow formats (25%), either with (8%) or without (17%) a superimposed
image of the narrator telling the story.
When both the book and the narrator were shown at the same time, the narrator was
typically seated on a sofa, bed, or chair (38.9%), sitting on the floor (2%), or standing
(6.1%). In these cases, viewers could hear the narration but could not see the book
in detail due to the distance from the camera or low resolution. The book was easier
to observe when the camera focused directly on it, though the narrator’s gestures
were less visible—something that, on the other hand, we did not analyze in this study.
Two types of productions were identified. The first, more elaborate and less common
(5.6%), used more than one camera and included post-production editing, allowing for
simultaneous shots of the picture book and the narrator. These videos often featured
credits or background music. The second, the more prevalent type of production (94.4%),
could be described as a homemade video, typically recorded with a phone or a static
camera in a single shot, or downloaded from a video call application.
Figure 1Visual narration technique and image quality
The videos published on YouTube were primarily aimed at viewers who were already subscribed
to the channel. As a result, narrators with a larger number of subscribers did not
begin directly with the story but instead included an introduction such as a greeting,
a welcome to their channel, or a brief overview of the topic they were about to cover.
This was the case in 30 of the videos. In these instances, there was a noticeable
tendency to pose questions about the story, and, given the asynchronous nature of
the narration, the mediators themselves would suggest possible initial hypotheses.
At the end of the narration, they often included comments and follow-up questions
for the viewers.
A significant part of the narrators (46%) read the verbal text from the picture book
without ever elaborating on it. They simply turned the pages, showing them either
fully or partially.
Figure 2Reading strategy
The literary text, as it appears in the original work, is reproduced entirety in all
the videos analyzed, without omitting a single word. However, only a few videos (19.2%)
display all the illustrations entirely, and in 5.7% of the videos, the book itself
is not shown at all; instead, only the narrator is visible while reading or dramatizing
the story. Therefore, while the literary text is always presented in its complete
form, the visual text is often only partially reproduced, as we will explore further.
Figure 3Strategies of omission or amplification
Regarding the peritext, the resulting verbal text often includes elements present
on the cover: the title was mentioned by all the narrators, and the author was mentioned
by 35 of them. However, this is not the case for the resulting visual text: in 7 of
the videos, the cover is not shown.
Figure 4The peritext (the cover)
The peritextual elements inside the book do not seem to garner the same attention.
Only 23 narrators showed the pages that appear before the first sentence. The other
30 narrators skipped showing the cover (which depicts the bear stepping off the boat),
the back cover (where the bear is seen waiting for the bus, above the dedication),
and the following page (where the bear enters a house). Instead, they opened the book
directly to the page containing the first sentence.
The verbal text is not usually amplified during these moments of the narration by
adding descriptive words about the actions depicted in the illustrations, nor do the
narrators draw attention to the visual elements—at least not with deictic expressions
or rhetorical questions prompting viewers to focus on illustrations that lack text.
None of the 30 narrators who did not show the peritextual pages described the actions
depicted there. Among the 23 narrators who did show the illustrations, only 7 made
any observations about what was seen in the pictures or began the narration from the
cover, while 16 started their narration by reading the first sentence.
As for the final peritext, the last page of the book, where the bear is seen parachuting
onto another house, was not shown in 20.8% of the cases. However, of the 42 remaining
videos that did show this page, only 12 expanded on the verbal text to describe what
was happening in the illustration.
Figure 5Visual reproduction of the initial and final peritext
This lack of attention does not extend to pages that contain both words and illustrations.
The words are never not read, and thus, pages with text are almost always shown, except
in the 5.7% of cases where the book is not shown at all.
Regarding whether the entire surface of the page is displayed—that is, if the viewer
is allowed to see all the details of the illustration—most videos (39) showed a fragmented
reproduction: The narrators did not display the entire image but rather zoomed in
on specific details, or the illustrations were not clearly visible due to the camera’s
distance from the book.
It is significant to observe what is hidden. 40 of the mediators show the full page
where the bear is hiding behind the screen, but in 13 videos (9 of which were narrated
in Spanish), this detail is not visible, either because the entire page is not shown
or because only details are presented, deliberately excluding the bear.
When amplifications occur in the resulting verbal text, they can happen on pages with
or without text and illustrations. For example, when the text says “and at night,
while William dreamed...” (), 15 narrators describe the actions of the bear. As for the initial peritext, the
narration was only amplified in 5 cases. Some of these amplifications were used to
interpret the story, offering opinions about the existence or nonexistence of the
bear that scares William. In the final peritext, only 12 of the mediators expanded
upon the image of the bear parachuting.
In interpreting Helen Cooper's picture book, we find notable differences. Among the
53 narrators, 13 conclude that the bear exists, 13 believe the bear is a product of
William’s imagination, and the remaining 27 do not take a clear position either way.
In some cases (videos 17 and 39), the explanations become contradictory. Certain scenes
are said to represent William's imagination, but when it comes to the images at the
end, it is interpreted that the bear leaves the house and heads toward another child's
house.
The 13 mediators who interpret that the bear exists amplified the narration both in
the peritext and in the threshold of the illustration. However, the other 13 who amplified
the narration to claim that the bear did not exist had hidden details from the illustration
in the resulting visual text, such as the image of the bear hiding behind the screen
or the images in the final peritext. The remaining 27 did not add any interpretation
in either direction.
The fundamental attitude, then, is to conceal details that complicate the reading.
But in a book where those details are found in the illustrations, the only way to
convey these contradictions to the audience is by pointing them out or amplifying
the text. This leads to two types of amplification, which we categorize as rich or
reductive.
Rich amplifications emerge to make sense of actions that are only shown in the illustration.
These amplifications attend to all the clues aimed at an attentive reader and challenge
the version given by the literary narrator, regardless of the conclusion drawn from
them.
This type of amplification appears in a small number of mediators (15), but when amplifications
occurred, they were more common than reductive ones (7), which will be discussed next.
An example of this is the continuation of the narration suggesting that the bear will
reach another child’s house, who, in turn, will also be afraid—explanations that point
to an open-ended conclusion. This contrasts with narrations that chose to ignore these
last pages in both the visual and verbal text, implying that the story ends when William
falls asleep and the bear disappears—forever, from his imagination.
Reducing amplifications refer to those interpretations in which details that challenge
the literary narrator’s version have been omitted. An example of this is in video
number 14, where the narrator emphasizes with words, tone of voice, and gestures while
reading the first page, that William was afraid of bears because he thought he had
seen one: “It was all because one day he thought—remember what I said—he thought [the emphasis is added to these words in italics] that he’d seen a bear there under
the stairs,” and she even underscores it with a comment, “Poor William, who thought
there was a bear under the stairs.” Furthermore, she anticipates from the beginning
that what William saw was not a bear, but rather “some furry material.”
Another mediator, driven by the need to discuss the irrational nature of the story,
directly addresses the viewer with a rhetorical question about the evident fictional
nature of the tale, emphasizing the absurdity of some actions, such as when the bear
draws a portrait of William.
Mediators who perform reductive amplifications seem to have intentionally concealed
the clues that could lead to alternative interpretations, showing only select images
and only in part (especially the detail of the bear hiding behind the screen in the
dark room or the illustrations where the bear is inside the house while the humans
are sleeping), so that these details align with their interpretation.
Others take advantage of the deliberate ambiguity of the phrase “And at night, while
William dreamed” () to explain that everything the bear was doing—and thus, the bear's very existence—is
part of William's dream.
Conclusions
This recourse to "sueñismo de ocasión" (), visible in amplifications that seek to finish the story with a rational interpretation,
is employed by several mediators who base their entire interpretation of the story
on this phrase and episode. For them, the actions depicting the bear flying to another
house serve to justify their own interpretation: the bear never existed; it was merely
a product of the child’s imagination and nightmares.
In some cases, the concealment of the initial and final peritexts or clues in the
illustrations removes any elements that contradict the interpretation the mediator
has constructed during their appropriation of the text.
In other cases, reductive amplification has been carried out despite the mediator
being attentive to details in the illustration that others had overlooked. These mediators
may be guided by their conceptions of what is acceptable or understandable for a child
reader, believing the child may be either unprepared for a complex story or too impressionable
for a narrative that could potentially frighten them.
This research suggests that some mediators who present picture books to an audience
through narration (whether digital or in-person, as the conclusions are clearly applicable
to live narrations) may be giving insufficient attention to the illustrations. This
lack of attention seems to stem from a limited familiarity with picture books, leading
them to apply knowledge based primarily on their experience with non-multimodal literary
texts. This could explain why some mediators jump straight from the cover to the page
with the first sentence, fail to pause at the final pages if they are located after
the last sentence, and do not recognize the peritext as part of the narrative. They
also fail to notice contradictions between the words and images, missing out on crucial
clues needed to construct meaning. Based on these findings, the particularities of
picture books appear to be insurmountable obstacles for some adult readers.
A revealing example of how adult and child readers interact differently with picture
books was verbalized by one narrator (number 33), who admitted that in a first reading,
she had believed the bear was not real, and it was her child who showed her otherwise.
These issues affect not only comprehension but also mediation practices. The materiality
of the book as an object offers the reader the possibility of becoming an interactive
reader of a text that resists linear reading (). This characteristic of the picture book can be reproduced or at least facilitated
in shared and joint reading practices between an adult mediator and a child reader.
However, the analyzed digital narrations are one-way and present not only a linear
reading trajectory but, in many cases, also an incomplete and fragmented one.
In digital narrations, it would be important to consider some of the recommendations
made for oral storytelling of picture books in real-life situations: not settling
for simply reading the verbal text of a picture book (; ), revisiting the peritext after reading (; ), and even interrupting a linear reading to go back and examine details already mentioned,
if the reading requires it.
On the other hand, the recommendation to maintain an open attitude toward the possible
interpretations that readers (especially young ones) may have () demands, at the very least, that in an asynchronous reproduction, significant parts
of the text should not be hidden but should be amplified or at least highlighted or
pointed out to encourage the reading process.
The research provides relevant insights into the significant role that adults play
in transmitting and, therefore, in reading and interpreting multimodal literary texts.
The outcome of mediation that is not attentive to the signals in the text may nullify
the work’s rich polysemy, denying young readers the opportunity to construct their
own meanings.
AUTHOR'S CONTRIBUTION
Cristina Quesada-Padrón: Project management; Formal analysis; Conceptualization; Data Curation; Writing -
original draft; Writing - proofreading and editing; Research; Methodology; Resources;
Supervision; Validation; Visualization.
Jesús Díaz-Armas: Project management; Formal analysis; Conceptualization; Data Curation; Writing -
original draft; Writing - proofreading and editing; Research; Methodology; Resources;
Supervision; Validation; Visualization.
Carmen A. Perdomo-López: Conceptualization; Data Curation; Research.
Patricia Mauclair: Conceptualization; Data Curation; Research.
References
1
Agosto, D. E. (1999). One and inseparable: interdependent storytelling in picture
storybooks. Children’s Literature in Education, 30, 267-280. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022471922077
Arizpe, E. (2022). Mediating the act of reading through picture books and fictional
readers. In A. M. Hagen (Ed.), Mediation and children's reading: Relationships, intervention, and organization from
the eighteenth Century to the present. Series: Studies in text and print culture. Lehigh University Press.
4
Arizpe, E., & Styles, M. (2002). ¿Cómo se lee una imagen? El desarrollo de la capacidad
visual y la lectura mediante libros ilustrados. Lectura y Vida, 23, 20- 29.
5
Bajour, C. (2022). Silencios y ruidos en diversas mediaciones sobre libros-álbum.
Catalejos. Revista sobre lectura, formación de lectores y literatura para niños, 14 (7), 52-64.
Cooper, H. (1993a). Hay un oso en el cuarto oscuro (C. Schreurer, trad.). Editorial Juventud.
11
Cooper, H. (1993b). The bear under the stairs. Corgi Books.
12
Díaz-Armas, J. (2003). Estrategias de desbordamiento en la ilustración de libros infantiles.
In F. L. Viana, M. Martins, & E. Coquet, Leitura, literatura infantil e ilustração. Investigação e prática docente. Centro de Estudos da Criança da Universidade do Minho. http://www.casadaleitura.org/portalbeta/bo/documentos/ot_jesus_diaz_a.pdf
13
Díaz-Armas, J. (2006). El contrato de lectura en el álbum: paratextos y desbordamiento
narrativo. Primeras noticias: Revista de Literatura, 222, 33-40. http://riull.ull.es/xmlui/handle/915/4466
14
Díaz-Armas, J. (2008). Lectura literaria y formación inicial: creencias del profesorado
en formación sobre el texto y su lector. In A. Mendoza Fillola (Coord.), Textos entre textos. Las conexiones textuales en la formación del lector (pp. 177-190). Horsori.
15
Díaz-Armas, J. (2010). La ilustración en lucha con la palabra. (Sobre algunas formas
de la ironía en el álbum). In El humor en la literatura infantil y juvenil: IV Congreso Internacional de la Asociación
Nacional de Investigación en Literatura Infantil y Juvenil (pp. 155-169). ANILIJ-Universidad de Cádiz.
Dueñas, J. D., Tabernero, R. M., Calvo, V., & Consejo, E. (2014). La lectura literaria
ante nuevos retos: canon y mediación en la trayectoria lectora de futuros profesores.
Ocnos, 11, 21–43. https://doi.org/10.18239/ocnos_2014.11.02
18
Duran, T., & Bosch, E. (2011). Before and after the picturebook frame: A typology
of endpapers. New Review of Children's Literature and Librarianship, 17(2), 122-143. https://doi.org/10.1080/13614541.2011.624927
19
Ellis, G., & Brewster, J. (2014). Tell it again! The storytelling handbook for primary English language teachers. British Council.
20
Ellis, G., & Mourão, S. (2021). Demystifying the read-aloud. English Teaching Professional, 136, 22-25.
21
Genette, G. (1982). Palimpsestes. La litterature en second degré. Seuil.
22
Genette, G. (1987). Seuils. Seuil.
23
Ghiso, M. P., & McGuire, C. E. (2007). “I talk them through it”: Teacher mediation
of picture books with sparse verbal text during whole‐class readalouds. Reading Research and Instruction, 46(4), 341–361. https://doi.org/10.1080/19388070709558475
24
González-Vázquez, M. S., & Gutiérrez-Sebastián, R. (2024). Mediación lectora docente
del álbum ilustrado. Estudio de caso. Didáctica. Lengua y Literatura, 36, 261-270. https://doi.org/10.5209/dill.90206
25
Gross, M., & Latham, D. (2017). The peritextual literacy framework: Using the functions
of peritext to support critical thinking. Library and Informations Science Research 39(2), 116-123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lisr.2017.03.006
26
Häggström, M. (2020). The art of read-aloud, body language and identity construction:
A multimodal interactional analysis of interaction between parent, child and picture
book. International Journal of Language Studies, 14(1), 117-140.
27
Harris, P. (2005). At the interface between reader and text: Devices in children's
picture books that mediate reader expectations and interpretations. AARE Annual Conference, Parramatta. https://www.aare.edu.au/data/publications/2005/har05606.pdf
28
Kümmerling-Meibauer, B. (1999). Metalinguistic awareness and the child ́s developing
concept of irony: The relationship between pictures and text in ironic picture books.
The Lion and The Unicorn, 23, 157-183. https://doi.org/10.1353/uni.1999.0024
29
Kunde, K. (2021). Narratives materials de l'àlbum Il·lustrat. Una aproximació intermedial a la didàctica
de da literatura [Dissertation, Universitat de Girona]. http://hdl.handle.net/10803/674753
30
Kunde, K., Arenas-Delgado, C., & Masgrau-Juanola, M. (2022). Los paratextos del álbum
ilustrado: la presencia de la función hermenéutica en los ganadores del Premio Internacional
Compostela. Journal of Literary Education, 6, 66-90. https://doi.org/10.7203/JLE.6.25357
Lewis, D. (2001). Reading contemporary picture books. Picturing text. Routledge.
33
Lluch-Crespo, G. (2009). Textos y paratextos en los libros infantiles. In J. I. Albentosa-Hernández,
P. C. Cerrillo-Torremocha, S. Yubero (Coords.), La formación de mediadores para la promoción de la lectura: contenidos de referencia
del Máster de Promoción de la lectura y literatura infantil. Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Castilla-La Mancha. http://www.cervantesvirtual.com/nd/ark:/59851/bmcdv216
Manresa, M. & Margallo, A. M. (2016). Prácticas de lectura en red: Exploración de
blogs literarios adolescentes. Catalejos. Revista sobre lectura, formación de lectores y literatura para niños, 2(3), 51-69. https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/articulo?codigo=9622677
36
Martínez, M., Stier, C. & Falcon, L. (2016). Judging a book by its cover: An investigation
of peritextual features in Caldecott Award Books. Children’s Literature in Education, 47, 225-241. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10583-016-9272-8
37
Mattos, M. S. (2016). As cintas e seus contratos de comunicação: envolvendo livro
e público leitor. Anais do III Seminário Internacional de Estudos sobre Discurso e Argumentação (III
SEDiAr).https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.34372.83844
38
Mattos, M. S., Ribeiro, P. F. N., & Vianna, S. (2016). Capas e contracapas de livros
ilustrados: espaços privilegiados de estratégias discursivas. Cadernos de Letras da UFF, 26(52), 349-372. https://doi.org/10.22409/cadletrasuff.2016n52a119
39
Montes, G. (2001). El corral de la infancia. Fondo de Cultura Económica.
Mourão, S. (2012). English picturebook illustrations and language development in early years education [Dissertation, Universidad de Aveiro].
42
Munita, F. (2021). Yo, mediador (a). Mediación y formación de lectores. Octaedro.
43
Muñoz, C., Munita, F., Valenzuela, J., & Riquelme, E. (2018). Profesores que leen
y enseñan a leer: El lugar de la lectura durante la formación. Revista Espacios, 39(40), 32-42. http://www.revistaespacios.com/a18v39n40/18394032.html
44
Nikolajeva, M., & Scott, C. (2001). How picture books work. Routledge.
Nodelman, P. (1988). Words about pictures. The narrative art of children’s picture books. University of Georgia Press. https://doi.org/10.1353/book26685
Pantaleo, S. (2010). Developing narrative competence through reading and writing metafictive
texts. Literacy Research and Instruction, 49(3), 264-281. https://doi.org/10.1080/19388070903100324
49
Phinney, T., & Colabucci, L. (2010). The best font for the job. Typography in Caldecott
Winners, 1990-2010. Children and Libraries, 8(3), 17-26.
50
Quesada-Padrón, C., Díaz-Armas, J., Perdomo-López, C. A., & Mauclair, P. (2024). Relación
de vídeos consultados para investigación sobre narraciones digitales de Hay un oso en el cuarto oscuro de Helen Cooper (1993). Zenodo. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.14022627
51
Ramos, A. M. (2016). On the liminality of picturebook: the case of mix-and-match books.
History of Education & Children's Literature, 11(1), 205-213. http://hdl.handle.net/10773/22071
52
Ramos, A. M. (2017). Segredos escondidos à vista de todos: implicações para a leitura
dos códigos de barras nos livros infantis. Aletria, Belo Horizonte, 27(2), 15-33. https://doi.org/10.17851/2317-2096.27.2.15-33
53
Ramos, A. M. (2019). The accordion format in the design of children’s books: A close
reading of a Portuguese collection. Libri & Liberi 8(2), 313-328. https://doi.org/10.21066/carcl.libri.8.2.4
54
Ramos, A. M. & Mattos, M. (2018). Revestir el libro de significados: un análisis de
las sobrecubiertas en la literatura infantil y juvenil. Ocnos, 17(2), 33-45. https://doi.org/10.18239/ocnos_2018.17.2.1679
55
Rhedin, U. (1992): Bilderboken- på väg mot en teori [El álbum ilustrado: de camino hacia una teoría]. Alfabeta.
Sipe, L. R. (2008). Young children’s visual meaning-making during read-alouds of picture
storybooks. In J. Flood, S. B. Heath, & D. Lapp (Eds.), Handbook of research on teaching literacy through the communicative and visual arts (Vol. 2). Routledge.
59
Sipe, L. R. & McGuire, C. E. (2006). Picturebook endpapers: Resources for literary
and aesthetic interpretation. Children’s Literature in Education, 37, 291-304. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10583-006-9007-3
60
Sipe, L. R. & McGuire, C. E. (2020). Picturebooks endpapers: Resources for literary
and aesthetic interpretation. In J. Evans, Talking beyond the page: Reading and responding to picture books (62-80). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003061151-6
61
Sotto-Mayor, G. (2016). As folhas de rosto nos livros ilustrados de literatura infantojuvenil:
uma proposta tipológica. Tropelías. Revista de Teoría de la Literatura y Literatura Comparada, 25, 335-352. https://doi.org/10.26754/ojs_tropelias/tropelias.2016251120
62
Tabernero-Sala, R. (2016). Los epitextos virtuales en la difusión del libro infantil:
Hacia una poética del book-trailer. Un modelo de análisis. Ocnos, 15(2), 21-36. https://doi.org/10.18239/ocnos_2016.15.2.1125
63
Tabernero-Sala, R. (2018). Leer el álbum desde la materialidad. La manipulación como
estrategia discursiva. In R. Tabernero (Ed.). Arte y oficio de leer obras infantiles. Investigaciones sobre lectores, mediación
y discurso literario (pp. 75-85). Octaedro.
64
Timpany, C., Vanderschantz, N., Hinze, A., Cunningham, S. J. & Wright, K. (2014).
Shared reading of children’s interactive picture books. In K. Tuamsuk, A. Jatowti,
& E. Rasmussen (Eds). The emergence of digital libraries – Research and practices (pp. 196-207). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-12823-8_20
Sipe, L. R., & Brightman, A. E. (2009). Young Children’s Interpretations of Page Breaks
in Contemporary Picture Storybooks. Journal of Literacy Research, 41(1), 68-103. https://dergipark.org.tr/en/pub/jef/issue/18638/196718