The aim of this study was to test the possible impact of the form information in presented (reading vs. listening situation) on the comprehension of narrative texts in students with ADHD comorbid with dyslexia. An experimental group with a mean age of 8.5 years and a control group with a mean age of 8.9 years participated. Three measures were used to analyse comprehension: lexical diversity, cohesive resources and episodic structure. The results showed no significant differences in the control group on all the measures under study, but there were significant differences in some measures in the experimental group, so that the use of cohesive resources and lexical diversity were favoured by the reading situation. Measures of episodic structure were not significantly affected by presentation modality.
Article Details
How to Cite
Vieiro-Iglesias, P., & González-Fernández, M. D. (2024). Reading and listening comprehension in the narrative of students with ADHD comorbid with Dyslexia. Ocnos. Journal of reading research, 24(1). https://doi.org/10.18239/ocnos_2025.24.1.494
Vieiro-Iglesias and González-Fernández: Reading and listening comprehension in the narrative of students with ADHD comorbid
with dyslexia
INTRODUCTION
Dyslexia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are two disorders that
are widely diagnosed in childhood, with more than 80% of children with ADHD and 60%
of children with dyslexia meeting the criteria for at least one additional diagnosis
(; ; ).
Dyslexia, in the DSM-5, is described as a specific learning disorder within the neurodevelopmental
disorders. People with dyslexia have persistent reading difficulty that cannot be
explained by sensory deficits, cognitive difficulties, lack of motivation or lack
of reading instruction. It is an unexpected, specific and persistent failure to acquire
efficient reading skills despite conventional instruction, adequate intelligence and
sociocultural opportunity ().
ADHD is one of the most prevalent developmental disorders, characterized by excessive
activity accompanied by low attention span and impulsivity, and is considered a psychiatric
pathology that has a continuum from childhood to adulthood. ADHD occurs in most cultures
in approximately 5% of children and 2.5% of adults ().
ADHD affects reading, which becomes difficult when intense activity is required for
long periods of time.
Attention and learning problems are generally considered to be interrelated and present
a developmental continuum (; ) and bidirectional, with comorbidity being very high, as is the case for children
with dyslexia for ADHD (; ) or children with ADHD for dyslexia (; Sánchez-Carmona et al., 2020; ) or children with ADHD for dyslexia (; ; ; ). Reading difficulties seem to be strongly related to the predominantly inattentive
type of ADHD and less so to cases of hyperactivity or impulsivity (; ; ).
Both behavioural and molecular genetic studies support a partially shared genetic
aetiology between ADHD and dyslexia and draw the comorbid phenotype as a result of
overlapping risk factors leading to a high rate of co-occurrence of these disorders.
It appears that children with ADHD and dyslexia have difficulty with different aspects
of information processing and in particular with memory-related functions. argue that comorbid subjects differ from ADHD in information processing characteristics,
although they are very close to dyslexics in the sense that they show deficits in
the word decoding process. Such difficulty seems to occur, especially in identifying
unfamiliar words (; ). These encoding problems can interfere with reading comprehension, making it difficult
to determine whether comprehension problems are due to decoding difficulties, comprehension
difficulties, or both (). At the comprehension level, there are deficits in the identification of topics
or main ideas, in the ability to make inferences, identification of textual inconsistencies,
ability to order fragments, and in planning and self-regulation skills (; ; ; ).
Due to the close relationship between lexical access and reading comprehension processes,
some studies have used the listening versus reading situation to explore comprehension
deficits associated with ADHD (; ; Lorch et al., 2020; ). Auditory comprehension studies show that people with ADHD also have difficulties
in higher-order language processes, such as understanding figurative language () and answering inferential questions (). In addition, they show deficits in organization, identification of main ideas and
coherence after listening to a text (; ; ).
If we think about how ADHD might affect comprehension processes, it seems that it
may strain the pool of cognitive resources available to form connections between text
ideas. Even when children with ADHD do not have comorbid word decoding problems, it
could be that their reduced attentional resources may affect the formation of text
connections, so that central information may not emerge in their text representation
to the same degree as it does for those without ADHD comprehension, leading to a centrality
deficit (). In terms of comprehension of texts that had been heard, it was found that, compared
to controls, children with ADHD showed a greater centrality deficit, even though they
recalled more central than peripheral information (). ADHD population appears to deplete the cognitive resources available to form connections
between text ideas, even when children with ADHD do not have comorbid word decoding
problems, have provided data supporting this idea in auditory comprehension tasks. Children
who had already retold passages they had heard or told about television programmes
they had seen showed this pattern.
In this context, it is interesting to know whether centrality deficits are also obtained
for reading comprehension. Thus, the purposes of this paper are (1) to know the influence
of the message reception modality (listening-reading) on the comprehension of narrative
texts (2) to examine the cognitive skills involved in this process such as episodic
organisation, cohesion and lexical diversity of text summaries.
We assume that there will be significant differences only in the measures of organisation
(episodic organization and cohesion), which will be favoured by the reading situation,
attributing to these a facilitating context for the retrieval of information as opposed
to the measures of lexical diversity more related to the level of previous vocabulary.
METHOD
Participants
This study initially consisted of 42 participants diagnosed with ADHD comorbid and
dyslexia, 21 girls and 21 boys. Once the Reading Delay Assessment test () was administered, which allowed us to match the sample at the level of Reading Age,
the sample consisted of 36 participants (16 girls and 20 boys). A further 36 subjects,
selected from a total of 40, formed the control group, half of whom were boys and
half girls, none of whom had learning difficulties or developmental disorders.
The sample presented a mental reading age corresponding to the same level of schooling
and trimester as established by the test, being in all cases between the 50th and 80th percentiles. The mean age of subjects with ADHD comorbid with dyslexia was 8.5 (SD
= 1.02), with 60th percentile on the reading task, 75th percentile on vocabulary and 75th percentile on the Raven intellectual development test. The subjects who acted as
controls had a mean age of 8.82 (SD = 1.59), with 75th percentile on the reading task, 75th percentile on vocabulary and 80th percentile on the Raven intellectual development test.
According to DSM-5 criteria, participants in the experimental group were classified
as ADHD only if symptoms were present before the age of seven and if these symptoms
caused significant functional impairment in two or more settings. All participants
met six or more symptoms on both dimensions (inattention and hyperactivity) which
resulted in them being identified as comorbid. Controls did not meet DSM-5 criteria
for either ADHD and/or dyslexia.
Since the selection of individuals was accidental and not random, in addition to ensuring
that the two groups were of the same size, special care was taken to ensure that their
composition was similar in terms of variables such as Age (F = 0.23; p = 0.79) and
Gender (c2 = 0.99; p = 0.71) so that the results could be contaminated as little as
possible.
Instruments and materials
- Reading Retardation Assessment Test_PEREL_ allowed us to match the sample in mental
reading age.
- Peabody Test, 3rd ed. (PPVT - III - Form A; ) to assess vocabulary level.
- For the assessment of IQ the was used, using reasoning by analogies, comparison of shapes and reasoning ability
based on figurative stimuli.
- The analysis of episodic structure, cohesive resources and lexical diversity was
carried out through an analysis of the summaries of two narrative texts.
Design
The design included the comparison of groups in order to analyse whether there were
differences in the implementation of comprehension strategies related to the modality
of presentation of the text on which the measures were analysed (episodic structure,
cohesive resources and lexical diversity). In both experimental situations, the subjects
had to produce a written summary of what they had read.
Episodic structure was measured by the percentage of propositions present in each
part of the text’s superstructure (Frame, Theme, Plot and Resolution) according to
Story Grammar ().
Cohesive resources (grammatical and lexical elements whose function is to unify the
different premises that make up a text) were assessed through the percentage of temporal,
causal, adversative, consecutive and conditional connectors present in the summaries.
Lexical diversity was measured through the use of different words in their summaries,
analysing the percentage of content words (nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs) and
function words present in the summaries.
Procedure
Once the consent of the families for the participation of the subjects in the study
was obtained, data collection was carried out at the educational centre attended by
the participants. This process was based on the practice of listening to and reading
narrative texts, with their subsequent comprehension in written form. The first text
is read by the researcher, and the participants then summarise it in writing, and
the second text is read by the participants, and they then summarise it in writing.
For the analysis of the summaries, an inter-judge agreement was made. Two experts
in reading comprehension analysed the superstructure of the summaries following the
methodology of . The total number of words and the number of different words were also counted in
order to measure lexical diversity, as well as the number and type of cohesive resources.
Cohen’s kappa was used to measure the agreement of the two raters. Two trials per
rater were used and the kappa value was .80.
RESULTS
The data obtained were analysed with IBM SPSS, v. 26.0. First, a descriptive analysis
was made of the results of the scores obtained in averages, maximums, minimums and
deviation in successes in the different measures in the control and experimental groups
(tables 1 to 6 and figure 1), where the differences between the control and experimental groups can be clearly
seen, with a higher level of effectiveness for the former in all the measures analysed:
lexical diversity, episodic structure, cohesive resources and identification of main
ideas. Likewise, no differences were found between the two experimental situations
in the control group, but there were differences in the experimental group.
Table 1Descriptive analysis of the experimental group on cohesive resources
Reading
Listen to
M
Max
Min
DT
M
Max
Min
Desvest
Temporary
46
56
32
7.53
54
60.00
43.00
5.28
Causes
32
35
23
6.61
45
68.00
41.00
6.42
Adversatives
5
7
3
1.93
0
0
0
0
Consecutive
2
6
1
1.87
0
0
0
0
Conditional
15
18
12
1.87
1
1
0
0.89
Table 2Descriptive analysis of the control group on cohesive resources
Reading
Listen to
M
Max
Min
DT
M
Max
Min
Desvest
Temporary
36
43
29
5.53
38
40
23
8.28
Causes
32
43
26
4.61
28
33
17
6.42
Adversatives
15
19
10
7.83
12
19
9
6.77
Consecutive
10
11
6
8.87
10
25
16
7.66
Conditional
17
19
8
3.87
12
29
8
8.77
The descriptive data showed that the control group used the greatest variety of cohesive
resources regardless of the experimental listening/reading situation. In both situations,
the highest percentage of resources used were temporal and causal, followed by conditionals,
adversatives and consecutives, which were in a lower range of use.
The experimental group used mostly temporal and causal connectors in both situations
(listening and reading).
The contrast of means analysing each of the measures of cohesive resources through
the Student’s t-test showed that the differences between the ADHD-dyslexia group and
the control were statistically significant on all measures in the listening situation
in favour of the latter (Temporal: t = 4.61, p = .001; Causal: t = 3.04, p < .003;
Adversative: t = 3.65, p = .004; Consecutive: t = 7.98, p = .005; Conditional: t =
8.71, p =.009 one-sided).
In the reading situation, significant differences were only found in favour of the
control group in the use of Temporal (t = 4.01, p = .009), Adversative (t = 1.047,
p = .03 and Consecutive (t = 3.66, p = .01; one-sided). In the ADHD-dyslexia group,
reading favoured conditional connectors, the differences with respect to the listening
situation reached statistical significance (t = 9.11, p = .041).
As for the analysis of lexical diversity, we first counted the use of different words
in the different summaries using the type-token ratio (TTR) technique. TTR is the
ratio obtained by dividing the types (the total number of different words) occurring
in a text or utterance by its tokens (the total number of words).
Figure 1 shows the mean number of same words (textual) and different words present in the
different experimental situations. It can be seen that the total number of words present
in each situation and group is similar, with no significant differences being found
either between the groups or comparing each of the situations (reading vs. listening).
However, the experimental group (ADHD-dyslexia) used a lower number of different words
in both situations than the control group, and these differences were significant
(Reading: t = 2.65; p = .001; Listening: t = 5.87, p = .003).
Figure 1Mean number of textual words and non-textual words in the different experimental situations.
The proportion of content words (nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs) and function
words present was also analysed.
Table 3Descriptive analysis of the experimental group on lexical diversity
Reading
Listen to
M
Max
Min
DT
M
Max
Min
DT
P. Function
40
46
32
7.83
33
43
32
9.58
Nouns
32
35
13
9.67
33
36
13
6.69
Verbs
19
28
9
7.77
20
28
10
7.60
Adjectives
8
11
6
8.98
10
11
5
8.97
Adverbs
1
3
0
0.72
2
6
0
4.76
Table 4Descriptive analysis of the control group in lexical diversity
Reading
Listen to
M
Max
Min
DT
M
Max
Min
DT
P. Function
29
36
22
8.76
29
20
13
5.58
Nouns
28
25
19
6.67
27
33
26
6.66
Verbs
27
31
25
7.88
28
31
21
6.60
Adjectives
10
14
6
8.97
9
14
4
9.97
Adverbs
6
10
3
4.77
7
9
6
1.76
The experimental group used a greater number of function words and nouns than the
control group. These differences reached statistical significance in the function
word measures (t = 11.56, p = .0001 and t= 12.65; p=.001 in reading and listening
respectively). The control group used more verbs, adjectives and adverbs in both situations,
reaching statistical significance in the measures of verbs in the reading situation
(t=12.76; p=.001) and adverbs in both situations (t=1.24, p=.003 one-sided).
The post-tests only showed significant differences in favour of the reading situation
in the use of function words in the ADHD-dyslexia group (t=12.88; p=.033).
As for the episodic structure analysed through the narrative superstructure, we observe
that the score pertaining to the percentage of recall of the different propositions
of each of the parts of the text is not significantly affected by the modality of
presentation in the experimental and control groups.
Table 5Descriptive analysis of the experimental group in episodic structure
Reading
Listening
M
Max
Min
DT
M
Max
Min
SD
Framework
30
46
19
7.83
35
43
32
9.58
Theme
5
9
1
9.67
3
6
1
6.69
Plot
40
48
29
7.77
42
48
38
7.60
Resolution
25
31
16
8.98
21
11
5
8.97
Table 6Descriptive analysis of the control group in episodic structure
Reading
Listening
M
Max
Min
DT
M
Max
Min
SD
Framework
21
26
.18
7.83
22
33
19
9.58
Theme
10
15
6
3.67
11
18
7
6.69
Plot
37
43
29
7.77
40
47
34
7.60
Resolution
32
41
26
8.98
27
31
22
8.97
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS
Children with associated disorders have more difficulties at the beginning of their
development, in terms of the learning process. In dyslexia, language acquisition may
be affected depending on the level of impairment.
In this case, we have worked with a group of ADHD comorbid with dyslexia in order
to test whether the format of presentation of oral/written information, i.e. reading
vs. listening situation, can influence the comprehension of narrative texts (a genre
with a familiar and clearly differentiated superstructure). We refer with to the importance
of the use of mechanisms such as causal, temporal or spatial connectors as linguistic
features which help the reader or listener to understand. In addition, the different
grammatical units and the variety of words (nouns, mental verbs, relative clauses,
adjectives and adverbs) present in the texts also justified our choice.
We used three measures to analyse narrative texts: lexical diversity, cohesive resources
and episodic structure. This analysis is based on the input-output of listening-writing
and reading-writing. The results showed that there were no significant differences
in the control group in all the measures under study, but there were significant differences
in the performance of the experimental group, from which it can be deduced that reading
comprehension difficulties do not characterise all the processes of this skill in
schoolchildren with ADHD-dyslexia, but that they are particularly evident in the use
of cohesive resources, as well as in lexical diversity.
The reading situation facilitated the use of cohesive resources and brought the experimental
group closer to the control group in the use of causal and conditional connectors.
Lexical diversity was similar in both groups, however, the control group made significantly
more use of content words than the experimental group. Also, the scores for the use
of verbs, adjectives and adverbs were significantly higher for the control group.
The experimental reading situation significantly benefited the use of function words
in the ADHD-dyslexia group.
Finding reduced competence in the use of textual coherence in listening situations
in subjects with ADHD-dyslexia adds to other work highlighting the convergence of
failures in listening comprehension (). Deficits in listening comprehension in ADHD subjects are interpreted as being more
likely to be due to failures in inhibition or poor task involvement (). These data lead us once again to the confirmation that the processing and comprehension
of auditory information by this group is deficient (; ; ; ).
In the reading and listening situations, the ADHD-dyslexia group performed at the
same level as the control group on measures of episodic structure. In this sense,
the percentage of remembered propositions, as well as the presence of the different
elements of the narrative superstructure did not discriminate the subjects according
to the experimental situation.
These results should be interpreted in the light of the demands of the text comprehension
task. Text comprehension involves a construction resulting from the representation
of the overall meaning of discourse. This process is relatively complex and interactive
in nature. In it, the individual brings into play different knowledge that engages
the limited storage and processing resources of working memory (; ), which is why we could interpret the cognitive overload involved in the listening
situation, as shown in the work of ; or ) who maintain that although the reading comprehension problems of children with attention
difficulties are related to poor word reading, listening comprehension is particularly
vulnerable in this type of population.
Along the same lines, maintain that reading favours comprehension over listening, as the visual presentation
of the text allows one to go back when something is unclear or when the thread of
the discourse is simply lost.
Once the results have been analysed in the light of the objectives set out, an open
line of research would involve the macro-structural analysis of the texts since, according
to , although the subject may have a complete literal understanding of the text, and
therefore complete its structure, ADHD subjects with dyslexia present difficulties
in interpretative comprehension, that is, the subject does not make inferences or
draw conclusions and also presents difficulties in critical reading, due to the fact
that they do not contribute any type of prior knowledge or add any assessment to the
text.
It can be concluded that the results of this research provide empirical evidence for
the hypothesis that there is a benefit of the reading situation among ADHD-dyslexia
profiles in measures of cohesive resources (mainly in the use of conditional connectors)
as well as a significantly higher use of function words, giving greater complexity
to the syntactic structure of the sentence, the latter aspect not analysed in previous
scientific literature.
However, the explanatory complexity underlying listening comprehension in ADHD will
need to be explored in depth in future work.
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
2
Beitchman, J. H., Hood, J., Rochon, J., Peterson, M., Mantini, T., & Majumdar, S.
(1989). Empirical classification of speech/language impairment in children I. Identification
of speech/language categories. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(1), 112-117. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-198901000-00021
3
Bellani, M., Moretti, A., Perlini, C., & Brambilla, P. (2011). Language disturbances
in ADHD. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 20(4), 311-315 https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796011000527
4
Bignell, S., & Cain, K. (2007). Pragmatic aspects of communication and language comprehension
in groups of children differentiated by teacher ratings of inattention and hyperactivity.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(4), 499-512. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151006X171343
5
Cain, K., & Bignell, S. (2014). Reading and listening comprehension and their relation
to inattention and hyperactivity. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(1), 108-124. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12009
6
Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., Adlof, S. M., & Barth, A. E. (2003, June). The simple
view of reading changes over time. In Annual Meeting of the Society for Scientific Study of Reading, Boulder, CO.
7
Daucourt, M. C., Erbeli, F., Little, C. W., Haughbrook, R., & Hart, S. A. (2020).
A meta-analytical review of the genetic and environmental correlations between reading
and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and reading and math. Scientific Studies of Reading, 24(1), 23-56. https://doi.org/10.1080/10888438.2019.1631827
8
Dhar, M., Been, P. H., Minderaa, R. B., & Althaus, M. (2008). Distinct information
processing characteristics in dyslexia and ADHD during a covert orienting task: An
event-related potential study. Clinical Neurophysiology, 119(9), 2011-2025. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2008.05.027
9
Dittman, C. K. (2016). Associations between inattention, hyperactivity and pre- reading
skills before and after formal reading instruction begins. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 29(9), 1771-1791. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9652-x
10
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., & Arribas, D. (2006). Peabody, test de vocabulario en imágenes. TEA Ediciones.
11
Ehm, J., Kerner auch Koerner, J., Gawrilow, C., Hasselhorn, M., & Schmiedek, F. (2016).
The Association of ADHD symptoms and reading acquisition during Elementary School
Years. Developmental Psychology, 52(9), 1445-1456. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000186
12
Erickson, L. C., Thiessen, E. D., Godwin, K. E., Dickerson, J. P., & Fisher, A. V.
(2015). Endogenously- and exogenously-driven selective sustained attention: Contributions
to Learning in Kindergarten Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 138, 126-134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.04.011
13
Flake, R. A., Lorch, E. P., & Milich, R. (2007). The effects of thematic importance
on story recall among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and comparison
children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(1), 43-53. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-006-9078-z
14
Gooch, D., Snowling, M., & Hulme, C. (2011). Time perception, phonological skills
and executive function in children with dyslexia and/or ADHD symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(2), 195-203. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02312.x
15
Haghshenas, S., Hosseini, M. S., & Aminjan, A. S. (2014). A possible correlation between
vestibular stimulation and auditory comprehension in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder. Psychology & Neuroscience, 7, 159-162. https://doi.org/10.3922/j.psns.2014.009
16
Herrera-Gutiérrez, E., Conesa-Conesa, M. R., & López-Ortuño, J. (2021). Estudio de
la comprensión lectora en alumnado con Trastorno por Déficit de Atención/Hiperactividad.
Investigaciones sobre Lectura, 15, 1-22. https://doi.org/10.24310/isl.vi15.12564
17
Hlas, A. C., Neyers, K., & Molitor, S. (2019). Measuring student attention in the
second language classroom. Language Teaching Research, 23(1), 107-125. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168817713766
18
Jarrett, M. A., Gilpin, A. T., Pierucci, J. M., & Rondon, A. T. (2015). Cognitive
and Reactive control processes: Associations with ADHA symptoms in preschoolers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(1), pp. 53-57. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025415575625
19
Kim, Y. G. (2016). Direct and mediated effects of language and cognitive skills on
comprehension of oral narrative texts (listening comprehension) for children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 101-120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.003
20
Leonard, M. A., Lorch, E. P., Milich, R., & Hagans, N. (2009). Parent - Child joint
picture-book reading among children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 12(4), 361-371. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054708315135
21
Lightbody, A. A. (2002). Effects of neurological processes on decoding, reading comprehension, and listening
comprehension in children with ADHD. University of California, Los Angeles.
22
Loh, P. R., Piek, J. P., & Barrett, N. C. (2011). Comorbid ADHD and DCD: Examining
cognitive functions using the WISC-IV. Research in developmental disabilities: A multidisciplinary journal, 32(4), 1260-1269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2011.02.008
23
Lorch, E. P., Milich, R., Polley-Sanchez, R., Van-den-Broek, P., Baer, S., Hooks,
K., Hartung, C., & Welsh, R. (2000). Comprehension of televised stories in boys with
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and nonreferred boys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(2), 321. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.109.2.321
24
Maldonado, A., Sebastián, E., & Soto, P. (1992). Prueba de evaluación del retraso en lectura (PEREL). Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
25
Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2007). Learning, attention, writing, and processing
speed in typical children and children with ADHD, autism, anxiety, depression, and
oppositional-defiant disorder. Child Neuropsychology, 13(6), 469-493. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040601112773
26
Mayes, S. D., Calhoun, S. L., & Crowell, E. W. (2000). Learning disabilities and ADHD:
Overlapping spectrum disorders. Journal of learning disabilities, 33(5), 417-424. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221940003300502
27
McInnes, A., Humphries, T., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock, R. (2003). Listening comprehension
and working memory are impaired in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder irrespective
of language impairment. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31(4), 427-443. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023895602957
28
Miller, A. C., Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F.,
& Olson, R. K. (2013). Reading comprehension in children with ADHD: Cognitive underpinnings
of the centrality deficit. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(3), 473-483. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9686-8
29
Miranda-Casas, A. (1988). Dificultades en el aprendizaje de la lectura, escritura y cálculo. Promolibro.
30
Miranda-Casas, A., Fernández-Andrés, M. I., García-Castellar, R., Roselló-Miranda,
B., & Colomer, C. (2011). Habilidades lingüísticas y ejecutivas en el Trastorno por
Déficit de Atención (TDAH) y en las Dificultades de Comprensión Lectora (DCL). Revista Psicothema, 23(4), 688-69. https://doi.org/10.14198/DCN.2017.4.1.07 ; http://hdl.handle.net/10234/36860
31
Moraleda-Sepúlveda, E., Pulido-García, N., & López-Resa, P. (2020). Dificultades de
lectoescritura en trastorno por déficit de atención e hiperactividad. Revista INFAD
de Psicología. International Journal of Developmental and Educational Psychology, 1(2), 211-222. https://doi.org/10.17060/ijodaep.2020.n2.v1.1972
32
Nilsen, E. S., Mangal, L., & MacDonald, K. (2013). Referencial communication in children
with ADHD: Challenges in the role of a listener. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 56(2), 590-603. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0013)
33
Purvis, K. L., & Tannock, R. (1997). Language abilities in children with attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder, reading disabilities, and normal controls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25(2), 133-144. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025731529006
34
Ramos-Puca, S. G. (2024). Revisión sistemática en niños con Trastorno de Déficit de
Atención con Hiperactividad en comprensión lectora. Ciencia Latina Revista Científica Multidisciplinar, 8(1), 10269-10288. https://doi.org/10.37811/cl_rcm.v8i1.10338
35
Raven, J. C. (1990). Test de matrices progresivas. Editorial Paidós.
36
Re, A. M. & Cornoldi, C. (2015). Spelling errors in text copying by children with
Dyslexia and ADHD Symptoms. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(1) 73-82. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219413491287
37
Sánchez-Carmona, A. J., Albert, J., López-Martín, S., Calleja-Pérez, B., Fernández-Mayoralas,
D. M., Jiménez-de-Domingo, A., Fernández-Perrone, A. L., & Fernández-Jaén, A. (2023).
El impacto del TDAH sobre la lectura. Medicina (Buenos Aires), 83, 22-26. https://www.scielo.org.ar/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0025-76802023000300022
38
Sanson, A., Prior, M., & Smart, D. (1996). Reading disabilities with and without behaviour
problems at 7–8 years: prediction from longitudinal data from infancy to 6 years.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(5), 529-541. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01439.x
39
Shalev, L., Kolodny, T., Shalev, N., & Mevorach, C. (2016). Attention functioning
among adolescents with multiple learning, attentional, behavioral and emotional difficulties.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(6), 582-596. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219415579125
40
Swanson, E., Barnes, M., Fall, A. M., & Roberts, G (2018). Predictors of reading comprehension
among struggling readers who exhibit differing levels of inattention and hyperactivity.
Reading & Writing Quarterly, 34(2), 132-146. https://doi.org/10.1080/10573569.2017.1359712
41
Thorndyke, P. W. (1977). Cognitive structures in comprehension and memory of narrative
discourse. Cognitive psychology, 9(1), 77-110. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(77)90005-6
42
Tinius, T. P. (2003). The integrated visual and auditory continuous performance test
as a neuropsychological measure. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 18(5), 439-454. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/18.5.439
43
Willcutt, E. G., & Pennington, B. F. (2000). Psychiatric comorbidity in children and
adolescents with reading disability. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(8), 1039-1048. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00691
44
Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., & DeFries, J. C. (2000). Twin study of the etiology
of comorbidity between reading disability and attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
American Journal of medical genetics, 96(3), 293-301. https://doi.org/10.1002/1096-8628(20000612)96:3293::AID-AJMG123.0.CO;2-C
45
Willcutt, E. G., & Petrill, S. A. (2023). Comorbidity between reading disability and
attention‐deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a community sample: Implications for academic,
social, and neuropsychological functioning. Mind, Brain, and Education, 17(4), 338-348. https://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12393